Introduction

Poor adherence undoubtedly contributes to glaucoma remaining the third most common cause of blindness in Western populations.1, 2, 3 Adherence issues are complex, Tsai et al4 classifying as many as 71 adherence barriers into four categories—regimen, individual patient factors, medical provider issues, and situational factors.

Despite the recent call for further research relating to adherence with glaucoma therapy5 and growing acceptance/encouragement for the use of qualitative methods,6 there has been only one previous study performed from an in-depth qualitative perspective7—somewhat surprising considering that assessment of adherence barriers relies primarily on patient's thoughts.

Taylor et al7 identified certain barriers to adherence for US patients, but the results were inevitably, culturally bound, and cultural factors influence adherence.2 Although many aspects relating to adherence may be similar in the United Kingdom and the United States, elements of cultural disparity and structural differences between health-care systems may affect health beliefs, attitudes, and hence, adherence. Although it was not the intention of the present study to examine explicit cultural differences between the United States and the United Kingdom, these differences highlight the value in conducting a UK study, similar to that of Taylor et al.7 Furthermore, Taylor et al7 highlighted the need for identification of motivational factors for adhering to glaucoma medication, an aim of the present study.

Materials and methods

Participants were recruited equally from two differing National Health Service (NHS) ophthalmic departments: the Norfolk and Norwich University Hospital (a teaching hospital serving urban and rural patients) and Cromer and District Hospital (serving rural patients). Participants either attended a focus group or had an interview. In total, 24 participants were recruited—7 to each focus group and 10 to interviews. The selected inclusion criteria (Table 1) ensured that participants had a significant clinical reason for adherence and were administering 2 medications to increase the ‘adherence effect’ and had used drops for 1 year to enable analysis of longer-term behaviour.

Table 1 Inclusion/exclusion criteria

Two focus groups were conducted, one at each hospital. Participants attended the focus group at their own hospital, held in a room away from the clinic for both comfort and to ‘dilute’ the effect of the clinic's environment. The focus groups were used to stimulate interaction between participants by asking open questions about experiences with therapy (Figure 1) so they could share/compare opinions and discuss any important areas not previously identified. The group meetings lasted for 2 h (longer than intended). However, concentration was well maintained, leading to an abundance of high-quality data. The information gathered from the focus groups ensured that all significant issues were incorporated into the interview guide, so that it reflected both theory and participant opinion (eg ‘education’ was added, appearing as an area of concern).

Figure 1
figure 1

Interview guide. The interview schedule was loosely based on that used by Taylor et al7 with the addition of questions concerning lack of symptoms, motivations, and ideas to help future adherence. Questioning followed a narrative approach whereby patients initially discussed their pathway to diagnosis and glaucoma background followed by the delivery of key questions to determine attitudes/behaviours towards adherence and motivations for adherence. Finally, the patients were given the opportunity to suggest their ideas for improved adherence.

Following the focus groups, 10 one-to-one, home-based, semi-structured, in-depth interviews of 40–90 min were held, allowing collection of specific thoughts and feelings from individual participants. The interview guide (Figure 1) provided only a basis for discussion. All interviews were conducted (and taped) by one psychologist (JL), who worked in isolation from the clinic staff. Anonymity of involvement and interview dialogues from clinic staff was emphasized to allow ‘free’ discussions.

The written transcripts were sent to all participants for ‘member checking’ as described by Lincoln and Guba.8 Participants were invited to amend/remove any statements (within 10 days). Only a few minor alterations were received (all being added to the analysis), confirming data reliability. The final data consisted of interview audio tapes and transcripts (81 463 words), researcher notes, and member-checking correspondence. Data were annotated with patient initials ensuring anonymity.

The study had Suffolk Research Ethics Committee approval, followed all applicable institutional/governmental regulations concerning ethical use of human volunteers and adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided written consent.

Analysis

Data were analysed using ‘Framework’ analysis,9 a technique not requiring saturation of data, which is useful from both diagnostic (examining the reasons for, or causes of, what exists) and strategic objectives (identifying new theories, policies, plans or actions) that reflected study aims. The primary analysis, performed by the principal investigator (JL), was validated by a co-investigator (HC), as recommended by Armstrong.10 NVivo7 Software (QSR International Pty Ltd., Doncaster, Australia) was used to organize and reliability-check data.

Results

Participant demography is summarized in Figure 2.

Figure 2
figure 2

Recruitment demography. Recruitment demography shown as a flow chart.

Six main themes emerged from the transcripts, which explained obstacles to adherence. Details for each theme are provided below using quotations from the interviews (Table 2), each barrier being derived directly from participant experiences.

Table 2 Table of quotes

Lack of education

Few participants appeared fully satisfied with the level of glaucoma education that they had received. Both at diagnosis and follow-up, the provision of leaflets was the commonest method utilized, but this was felt by many to be inadequate, the desire being for more discussion about treatment. Other participants had received help on a request-only-basis and a few reported receiving no help.

Unsatisfactory hospital-led education was considered to be the direct result of the current NHS system, where doctors appeared too busy clinically to have time to provide adequate education. Furthermore, it appeared that doctor–patient communication issues such as the overuse of medical terminology or the use of different drop names often caused confusion. Many participants had self-educated using pharmacy leaflets, articles, pharmaceutical industry information or websites (quote 1). However, key knowledge about glaucoma still appeared lacking for two participants despite experience of the disease for >1 year (one participant reported being unaware whether or not glaucoma was fatal, having known someone who died shortly after trabeculectomy). Some participants also discussed how they felt common misconceptions about glaucoma within the community needed addressing.

In light of these issues, many participants expressed a desire for improved delivery of education. Participants emphasized the need for advice at diagnosis and during the first few consultations, both to relieve worry/shock and to provide correct information so that they might self-manage effectively from diagnosis (quote 2).

Participants discussed how education should concentrate on the consequences of poor adherence, drop application techniques, and advice about application schedules. Many proposed the introduction of a trained education assistant as a feasible way of providing advice, potentially improving adherence, and saving clinician time (quote 3). Participants also discussed education throughout follow-up as an area requiring expansion (quote 4).

Lack of faith in drop efficacy

Many participants had certain negative feelings about drops: they were ‘paraphernalia’ (EM) and the side effects could be unpleasant. However, it appeared that despite these issues, the majority continued to administer drops due to faith in drop efficacy preserving sight (quote 5), or because they already had symptomatic visual loss (quote 6). Understandably, sight preservation appeared to be the major motivation for adherence. All participants were emotional when imagining blindness and the anticipated effect of this for themselves/their families (quote 7).

However, it appeared that when efficacy or safety was doubted, motivation to persevere diminished. One participant described how he sometimes ‘chose’ not to administer drops (quote 8). Anticipated long-term side effects led another participant to undergo a supervised trial off therapy to prove that the drops were effective. Many participants desired more regular feedback about drop efficacy, so providing them with greater faith for adherence.

Drop application issues

Many participants recalled problems with drop administration soon after diagnosis and several discussed current problems. Problems appeared most poignant in the initial stages, with many participants recalling that uncertainty about application methods and difficulty in physically administrating drops reduced confidence and application accuracy (quote 9).

Later in care, participant opinion over-drop application was split; for some drop administration became simple and ‘second-nature’ (TE), while for others there were continued concerns with respect to correct use of medication. Operational and technique issues provided the main areas of concern with uncertainties often remaining chronically (quote 10). For example, some participants reported finding it awkward to apply drops correctly, sometimes flooding the eye or face. Additional physical problems with grip, balance, and precision were highlighted by some older participants (quote 11).

Both initial and chronic application issues were considered to have arisen from poor early drop application education. Few participants had received any guidance with respect to effective administration (quote 12). As a result, participants had had to rely on instructions provided with their medication, often considered difficult to read, or confusing as it conflicted with information obtained from other sources (eg internet, pharmacists, and other patients). The need for drop administration advice at diagnosis was discussed far more than any other topic, with many participants proposing to ask their practitioner to check their current technique.

Forgetting drops

All but two of the participants admitted to forgetting drops at some stage. For the majority, the frequency of forgetting appeared to be split between those who occasionally forgot and those who rarely forgot. There were marked attitude differences between the feelings of those who frequently, and those who rarely, forgot. While the majority of frequent/occasional forgetters were in agreement that if they forgot they would not particularly worry (quote 13), most of the participants who never/rarely forgot appeared concerned (quote 14). In addition, focus-group participants appeared to admit to forgetting drops more freely after others had done so, many discussing this in a light-hearted manner. In contrast, interview participants tended to stick with their original comments.

For all participants, forgetting drops appeared to be explained by factors influencing routine. All reported a reliance on daily events such as bedtime or meals to act as cues. For some participants clock-time was their only cue. Events (eg holidays) that altered normal routine were the most commonly reported reasons for poor adherence (quote 15). In addition, particular doses, those prescribed to be taken at busiest times of day or doses that failed to fall naturally into a daily regimen, were more ‘at risk’ of being forgotten (quote 16). A complex dosing regimen with a greater quantity of doses, for example with the addition of post-operative drops, had an adverse effect (quote 17).

Several other cues and memory aids were described, including the assistance of a spouse, a chart or alarm. However, these methods all appeared to have been used in conjunction with habits and/or clock-time.

Practical problems

Participants discussed several practical issues acting as barriers to adherence. Some had been untreated for short periods having run-out of drops, failing to re-order drops or having experienced problems obtaining new drops before current bottle expiry. Others who had been prescribed drops to be taken during the day, rather than early-morning or late-evening, admitted adherence difficulties. Problems included difficulties in finding a convenient location (often due to embarrassment), having inadequate time to apply the drops, keeping drops cool, eyes watering/stinging while trying to perform daily activities, and remembering drops when busy (quote 18).

Several participants complained about medication packaging, side effects, and prescription costs (for employed participants, <60 years). However, although these issues were described as frustrating and potentially off-putting to participants, none were explicitly associated with non-adherence.

Age and individual differences

Both individual and age differences were applicable to all themes. By classifying participants as ‘older’ (60 years) or ‘younger’ (<60 years) and comparing attitudes, it became evident that ‘older’ patients more often required additional assistance, with many anticipating, or indeed experiencing, greater difficulty with physically applying and remembering drops (quote 19). Conditions associated with older age were also anticipated to reduce adherence (eg tremor, arthritis). Some older participants discussed difficulty in accessing external educational resources, particularly websites (quote 20). In contrast, many of the ‘younger’ participants felt cheated or depressed, considering glaucoma to be an ‘older person's disease’. Many discussed concern about life-long treatment (quote 21) and a feeling of isolation in clinics when surrounded by older patients. The desire for a ‘young-only’ glaucoma group where young patients could exchange specific age-related advice was widely suggested (quote 22). Furthermore, ‘younger’ participants felt they were treated differently by staff, with some feeling hospital staff were overly helpful, and others feeling they were less supportive. Age-related issues identified by younger participants emerged during interviews and not in the focus groups. As a taboo subject, age may not have been discussed in focus groups to avoid offence.

Individual differences were evident across all themes. Opinions differed according to educational needs, degree of initial, and ongoing application problems, tendency and feelings towards forgetting drops, and adherence cues (including routine). Further practical problems and age-related differences also varied with individual lifestyle. Participants widely discussed the idea of ‘tailoring’ management with respect to age and individual differences, both to maximize the use of clinical time and maximize adherence.

Discussion

The present study was the first UK-based, qualitative investigation of adherence with anti-glaucomatous therapy. Multiple obstacles to adherence were identified through an extensive ‘Framework’ analysis of participants’ thoughts/feelings, collected from both focus-groups and individual interviews. Lack of initial education about application techniques, consequences of non-adherence, and drop-scheduling issues were regarded as key factors. Patients appeared to need: sufficient initial information, long-term education (with feedback), and the establishment of sufficient medication-faith to enable self-management. Although the fundamental motivation to adhere was sight-preservation, two ‘exceptions’ demonstrated that adherence can lapse when drop efficacy is doubted. Difficulties with drop application, forgetting drops, and other practical issues including scheduling, as well as problems with drop supply, were other more common barriers to adherence. Furthermore, individual and age-related differences were identified for all obstacles; hence, the suggested ‘tailoring’ of adherence advice.

Many findings were similar to those of the Taylor et al7 US-based study. Since the majority of participants in both studies admitted non-adherence, with forgetfulness and application issues identified as two major problems, the importance of addressing these issues should be emphasized. Our study answered Taylor's question: ‘What factors motivate patients to be adherent and what could motivate them further?’7 Fear of blindness appeared to be the universal motivation for adherence, explaining why lack of faith in drop efficacy had a negative influence. The results highlighted the importance of providing feedback about medication efficacy and how a move away from an often inappropriate ‘fear of blindness’ driver to adherence would be ideal.

Adherence levels appeared reasonable for our group of patients (the majority forgetting drops infrequently), drop administration problems being resolved soon after diagnosis, and a large number of additional practical problems being overcome with time. Adherence in our study seemed to be linked to a common fear of visual loss, inducing self-motivated perseverance, identification of memory cues, and solutions to various practical/logistical problems. The fact that all participants administered drops using ‘routine’ was an example of such motivation.

However, since many participants experienced periods of poor adherence and recalled problems as newly diagnosed patients, changes in clinical practice appear necessary. Improving clinical practice is particularly important given adherence levels are currently underestimated by clinicians and patients.11, 12, 13 Our findings may be useful to both clinicians and patients in promoting adherence, identifying individual causes of non-adherence as early as possible after diagnosis and stimulating appropriate discussion to find potential solutions. Emphasis should be placed on identification of the stage(s) where adherence is breaking down for an individual patient so that tailored additional assistance and advice can be provided. Berger14 suggested the use of motivational interviewing to improve adherence; a move towards a psychosocial model of care (rather than the traditional biomedical approach). Future development of a health behaviour model specific to glaucoma patients should therefore combine both a promotional tool and treatment strategy aid for long-term management.

In addition, our results, like Taylor et al7 suggested that the introduction of an education assistant, working in glaucoma clinics would be a useful resource having the potential to combat obstacles to adherence. Further to Taylor et al,7 our results suggested that the emphasis of this assistance should be at diagnosis and should be tailored to the individual. Overall adherence levels could be improved by better initial education at diagnosis, together with long-term feedback at all stages. Understanding the consequences of non-adherence would appear to have the potential to establish motivation from the outset. Participants could be taught effective application techniques, inducing confidence. Memory aids/cues and the importance of routine could be discussed and administration schedules devised so that effective measures to ensure adherence are in place at commencement of treatment. Potential practical barriers would be addressed and overcome. Furthermore, by ‘tailoring’ education to the individual according to age and lifestyle, specific adherence issues would be targeted and potentially overcome in an appropriate and patient-centred manner.

Future research should focus on assessing the value of health behaviour models in improving glaucoma medication adherence and investigating methods by which initial education, drop application techniques, and advice on memory aids/cues should best be delivered to patients with glaucoma.

Methodological critique

From the rich amount of data acquired and the creation of six themes directly reflecting participant opinion, ‘Framework’ analysis and qualitative methodology successfully unearthed participant opinion. Use of both focus groups and interviews allowed the study to benefit from the dynamics attributed to both group discussion and individual interviews. Participants in the focus groups appeared encouraged by one another to admit to non-adherence, and yet the privacy of individual interviews, enabled discussion about age-related issues. In addition, the agreement between focus groups and interviews in terms of content and opinion added to study reliability. The acknowledgement and use of negative cases and accumulative techniques15 throughout the analysis increased validity and revealed the importance of diversity of opinion in the glaucoma patient population, which may have been overlooked using quantitative methods.

Qualitative research in particular can be criticized for being generalizable to only the population from which the participants were selected. The relationship between adherence and socio-demographic factors such as age, gender, race, and intelligence/education is complex, consequently random selection of participants was employed rather than selecting a sample in an attempt to represent the whole UK population. Unfortunately, due to low ethnic diversity and strict inclusion criterion (resulting in exclusion for clinical reasons of the few screened ethnic minority patients), all except one of the participants were white British (with one white-other). The authors acknowledge that their findings may not apply to all the UK residents since ethnicity affects adherence2 and would encourage further studies for different ethnic groups. However, apart from ethnicity, it was considered that the sample selected, provided a reasonable representation of the UK population with a wide age range, equal gender split, and variety of occupations that indicated different levels of education. Participants provided a broad representation of patient opinion and practical issues, relevant to the majority of the UK glaucoma patients under NHS care.

Reflexive analysis

Willig15 promoted the importance of both personal and epistemological reflexive analysis in qualitative research. On reflection ‘Framework’ analysis was considered appropriate in gaining an understanding of participants’ experiences. Although it was accepted that the principal investigator (JL) was important in theme creation, the theories identified were grounded in participants’ discourse, were co-investigator verified, and as such were considered a good indication of real thoughts/perceptions. Since participant diversity and novel experiences were revealed and not anticipated, concerns regarding such bias were minimized.

Studies of adherence may be intrinsically biased though the selection of patients who attend follow-up appointments; non-adherent patients who suffer the key problems of adherence and ongoing care being more likely to drop-out of follow-up and be missing from random selection. While the authors were unable to account for those patients who had dropped out of care, by noting the number of patients approached and explanatory reasons for non-participation (Figure 2), it can be seen that the majority of participants approached agreed to take part, this including some self-confessed poorly adherent participants who contributed to the findings. However, since the sample was theoretically the more adherent patients (those attending follow-up and agreeing to participate), the magnitude of adherence concern is likely to be under-reported—thus, caution is warranted in extrapolating these results.

Participants may have felt uneasy about admitting non-adherence. Although anonymity from clinical staff was guaranteed and the principal investigator was a non-medically trained health professional, non-adherence may have been under-reported. However, in view of the large amount of data collected, a good rapport throughout all interviews and the admission by many of non-adherence, our findings were considered to be valid. The focus group and interview locations were away from the clinic environment, facilitating the process and ‘diluting’ clinician presence. Finally, many participants reported greater awareness of adherence problems and approved of the opportunity to discuss their concerns demonstrating how the participants themselves benefited from the study.

Conclusion

Multiple barriers to adherence with anti-glaucomatous therapy were classified, which may aid their identification in future clinical practice. Findings suggest that specific obstacles to adherence should be identified soon after diagnosis and patients educated in how to tackle them. Use of health behaviour models and introduction of a glaucoma education assistant may facilitate the development of a tailored approach, accounting for individual and age-related differences in preventing poor adherence. Future research should focus on investigating methods by which initial education about glaucoma and its management, should best be delivered to patients.