Article Text

Download PDFPDF

Clinical science
Saccade testing to distinguish between non-organic and organic visual-field restriction
Free
  1. M S Zinkernagel1,
  2. N Pellanda1,
  3. A Kunz1,
  4. D S Mojon1,2
  1. 1
    Department of Neuro-Ophthalmology and Strabismology, Kantonsspital, St Gallen, Switzerland
  2. 2
    University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland
  1. Correspondence to Professor D S Mojon, Department of Neuro-Ophthalmology and Strabismology, Kantonsspital, 9007 St Gallen, Switzerland; daniel.mojon{at}kssg.ch

Abstract

Aim: The authors described and tested a simple bedside procedure to detect non-organic visual-field loss.

Methods: Prospective comparative observational case series of 16 patients with non-organic visual-field loss and 15 patients with organic visual-field loss were examined. Saccade patterns provoked by a stimulus outside the claimed visual field were assessed by a masked observer.

Results: Whereas, in organic visual-field defects, eye movements as noted by the observer were in small and erratic searching patterns towards the visual-field defect in all patients (15/15), most patients with non-organic visual-field loss (14/16) were able to jump directly to the presented red cap in one directional large saccade, although the stimulus was outside their stated visual field. The sensitivity of the saccade test in detection of non-organic visual-field loss by a masked observer was 87% (95% CI 60% to 97%) and the specificity was 100% (95% CI 75% to 100%). The positive predictive value for non-organic visual-field loss of the saccade test was 100%, and the negative predictive value was 90%.

Conclusions: The saccade test is a quick and reproducible examination to use and is largely independent of the patient’s willingness for cooperation. The authors believe that the test will be of value to clinicians on bedside evaluation when non-organic visual-field loss is suspected.

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Non-organic visual-field loss is occasionally encountered by the ophthalmologist. It can be caused deliberately by feigning visual-field loss, so-called malingering, by a subconscious process, defined as ocular conversion disorder,1 lack of cooperation, somatisation disorders or psychiatric diagnoses.

Whatever the underlying cause, a high index of suspicion is needed to consider this condition, and it is often only after extensive neurological and ophthalmological workup and documentation that malingering or a psychogenic cause may be detected.2 Thus, to prevent excessive medical investigations and shorten the period of work disability, it is useful to have tests which are easy to perform and reproducible, and have a high specifity to point towards this condition. Even if all the signs suggest a non-organic aetiology, it has to be kept in mind that in some cases non-organic disease may coexist with organic disease.3

Whereas a normal subject is able to calculate the distance to fixate an object recognised in the peripheral visual field and thus can trigger directional saccades, patients with hemianopsia or constricted visual fields are not able to localise the target stimulus in their retinal eccentricity and thus will display a searching eye movement pattern until they find the target.4 5 Most malingerers or patients with psychogenic visual-field loss, however, do not realise that testing the accuracy of their saccades corresponds to the function of their visual field and display a high accuracy in their saccadic eye movements, although the target is in a supposedly blind area.

Since saccadic eye movements can usually be observed with the naked eye, they allow insight into the patient’s perceptual visual-field state. Although the test has been described in general ophthalmology textbooks on non-organic visual loss,6 there is, to our knowledge, no study validating its sensitivity and specificity. In order to address this issue, we sought to determine whether observation of saccade patterns allows differentiation between organic and non-organic visual-field defects by an masked observer and validation of the sensitivity and specificity of this test in a prospective study.

Material and methods

The research followed the Tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. It was considered to inform the patients about the study. However, this might have alerted the behaviour of the patients with non-organic visual-field loss. Therefore, and because of the completely non-invasive and short nature of the saccade testing, the ethical committee permitted the examination to be performed without the consent of the patient.

Inclusion criteria included consecutive patients seen by DSM in the years 2006 and 2007 in his private practice at the Kantonsspital St Gallen, having Goldmann kinetic perimetry for unclear visual-field defects. Since most of these patients were referrals from other ophthalmologists, the proportion of non-organic visual-field loss was not representative for typical ophthalmological demographics. Patients with organic visual-field loss were included if they had a structural abnormality in neuroimaging or eye examination explaining the pattern of visual-field loss.

Patients were defined as having non-organic visual-field loss if they had a normal structural eye exam, no abnormality on neuroimaging or electrophysiological testing to support an organic aetiology for the visual-field loss and/or evidence for non-organic visual loss through testing other than the saccadic testing. Standard full-field ERG and VEP were carried out in 69% (11/16) of cases with non-organic visual-field defects. MRI testing was performed in 62% (10/16) of patients. Some patients (2/16, 12.5%) did not have electrophysiological testing because Goldmann kinetic perimetry and history pointed unambiguously towards non-organic visual-field loss.

The “gold standard” for diagnosis of non-organic visual-field loss was assured by testing the visual field at various distances. Kinetic Goldmann perimetry (background luminance 10 cd/m2) with various stimulus luminances according to the visual acuity was done in the usual fashion with the stimulus moved in from the periphery at the standard distance of 30 cm. Then, the same procedure with the same settings was repeated at a double distance of 60 cm. In patients with non-organic visual-field defects, the radius of the visual field was not significantly larger or even smaller when tested at double distance. In addition, spiral-shaped or tunnel-shaped visual fields in Goldmann perimetry were used as a criterion for diagnosis of non-organic visual-field loss.

In addition, spiral-shaped or tunnel-shaped visual fields in Goldmann perimetry were used as a criterion for diagnosis of non-organic visual-field loss.

Exclusion criteria for non-organic visual-field loss included an abnormal structural eye exam that could explain visual-field loss or abnormality on neuroimaging or electrophysiological testing in support of an organic aetiology for the visual-field loss or lack of a second test supporting non-organic visual-field loss. Exclusion criteria for patients with organic visual-field loss were: (1) inadequate clinical investigation and documentation, (2) insufficient evidence for organic visual loss or (3) patients who were not alert and oriented.

All saccade tests were performed by the same investigator (DSM), in order to reduce the testing procedure variability. He was aware of the final diagnosis, organic or non-organic visual-field loss, and had seen the results of the Goldmann visual-field testing or a global constriction of the visual field. In addition, a masked observer was present to evaluate the saccade pattern.

The examiner was sitting in front of the seated patient at a distance of about 60 cm, holding in each hand a bottle of eye-drops with a red cap. All tests were performed binocularly, because all examined patients had binocular visual-field defects. The examiner’s wrists were placed at the patient’s eye level about 30° to the right and left in a plane approximately 50 cm away from the patient (fig 1). The patient was informed about the test in the following way:

Figure 1

Test arrangement for saccade testing. The examiner’s wrists were placed at the patient’s eye level about 30° to the right and left in a plane approximately 50 cm away from the patient.

Now we will retest your eye movements, which were already previously found to be normal. I will show you a red bottle cap to your right. Please never move your head, just look at the target. Then, when I say “left,” please look at the red bottle cap to your left. Continue to look at the left bottle cap until I say “right.” This procedure will be repeated several times.

While the patient was looking at one target, the position of the other bottle remained unchanged or not. If the position was changed, the wrist and arm were kept in the same position, and only the hand was flipped either up or down, depending on the actual bottle position (fig 2). This is very important, since any movement of the arm might be detected by an intact visual field and, thus, could represent an indirect indication as to where the target has been moved. Saccade testing was repeated several times, and the patient had at least 2 s to rest on one target before refixating the other target. To provide a masked observation, a third person, unaware of the patient’s diagnosis, was observing the saccades between the four possible positions. A non-organic saccadic response in our study was defined as a single, accurate, and direct saccade to the presented target in the supposedly blind visual field as judged by an independent observer. An organic saccadic response was defined as an inaccurate or multistep searching movement towards a target in the blind field. The masked observer then judged whether all saccades were directional and accurate towards all stimuli demonstrating an intact visual field or if the saccades were in an erratic searching pattern, indicating a visual-field defect. If an abnormal visual field was suspected, the observer had to state which of the following field defects was suspected: quadrant (and if so which one), hemianopsia (and if so which part), only one quadrant intact (and if so which one), or a global constriction of the visual field. Only the result of the masked observer, but not the opinion of the person performing the saccade test (DSM), was used for analysis.

Figure 2

Test arrangement for saccade testing. While the patient was looking at one target, the position of the other target remained unchanged or not. If the position was changed, the wrist and arm were kept in the same position, and only the hand was flipped either up or down.

Results

Thirty-one consecutive patients with visual-field restriction referred to the Neuro-Ophthalmological Service at the Kantonsspital St Gallen, Switzerland, between 2006 and 2007 were included. Visual-field defects were confirmed by standard Goldmann perimetry. Sixteen patients had non-organic visual-field loss (mean age: 41 years). Fifteen patients had organic visual-field loss (mean age: 40 years). All non-organic visual-field defects were concentric to 10° of remaining visual field or less. Patients with organic visual-field loss had either quadrant or hemianopsia (12 patients), with absolute visual-field defects in that quadrant or hemifield, or concentric visual-field loss to 10° of remaining visual field or less (three patients).

All patients had a thorough neuro-ophthalmological examination with funduscopy, biomicroscopy, ocular motility and pupil evaluation.

The mean visual acuity in the group with organic visual defects was 0.8 OD and 0.85 OS (range best-corrected visual acuity of the better eye (BCVA) in decimal fraction: 0.2–1.25), and in the group with non-organic visual-field loss, the mean visual acuity was 0.64 OD and 0.62 OS (BCVA: 0.1–1.25).

All patients (15/15) with organic visual-field defects displayed at least two saccades towards the presented target and usually showed small and erratic searching patterns towards the visual-field defect. In contrast, saccadic eye movements towards the intact visual field were accurate. This searching pattern was found, whether patients had a truly constricted visual field or hemianopsia and regardless of the underlying organic condition, as long as the presented stimulus was outside their visual field. In contrast 14 out of 16 patients (87%) with non-organic visual-field loss were able to jump directly to the presented finger in one directional large saccade, although the stimulus was outside their stated visual field. Two out of 16 patients (13%) with non-organic visual-field loss displayed saccadic patterns which were considered as erratic by the observer.

These two saccade patterns were highly reproducible in either group and appeared to be independent of visual acuity, as long as the presented stimulus could be recognised. The nature of the visual-field defect (i.e constrictive, quadrantic or hemifield) was identified correctly by the masked observer in all cases. It is important that the test be conducted using four different hand positions by the examiner (ie, left up and down and right up and down). Only with this method would the masked observer be able to differentiate between quadrantic or hemianopsia or constricted visual fields.

The sensitivity of the saccade test in detection of non-organic visual-field loss was 87% (95%CI 60–97%) and the specificity was 100% (95%CI 75–100%). The positive predictive value for non-organic visual-field loss of the saccade test was 100%, and the negative predictive value was 90%. Additionally three patients in this group displayed a spiral-shaped visual field highly suggestive of a non-organic constricted visual field.

Discussion

Diagnosis of non-organic visual-field loss may be very difficult, especially when patients are malingering, have had multiple prior examinations or have rehearsed their visual-field defect.7 It has been shown that, with minimal coaching, healthy subjects can imitate quadrantic, hemianopic and altitudinal visual-field defects in both automated and manual perimetry.8 In most cases, however, non-organic visual-field defects are bilateral and concentric, because concentric defects appear to be easier to simulate. In addition, non-organic visual-field defects often present with other ocular manifestations of non-organic ocular disease. These include first and foremost loss of visual acuity9 10 but also paralysis of horizontal and vertical gaze, monocular diplopia, ptosis, blepharospasm, nystagmus, convergence and accommodation insufficiency, and spasms of the near reflex.

Whereas numerous special tests exist to assess non-organic loss of visual acuity,11 there are few tests to assess non-organic visual-field restriction. A good test to convey non-organic visual field constriction is to repeat the Goldmann perimetry at double the original distance. Whereas most patients with an organically constricted visual field will display an increase in the absolute size of the visual field, patients with a non-organically constricted visual field will maintain about the same absolute size of their visual field. Monocular hemianopsia persisting on binocular testing will also arouse suspicion.

Although saccade testing is well known by many ophthalmologists and is described in general ophthalmology textbooks on non-organic visual loss, there is no report to validate its specificity and sensitivity.

We revisit this quick and reproducible test to use when non-organic visual-field defects are suspected and which is largely independent of the patient’s willingness for cooperation and determined its sensitivity and specificity in a prospective clinical study. The test makes use of the fact that erratic saccades are very difficult to simulate, and directional saccades are very hard to suppress. Thus, observation of triggered saccades allows a statement about the visual field beyond the claimed limits.

An important advantage of this test compared with others is that the patient is not suspicious that the visual field is tested. Often the patient will think that their eye movements and reaction time are examined. This feeling is reinforced by the examiner’s statement that “the patient’s eye movements, which were already previously found to be normal, will be retested.”

This command will help eliminate false-negative answers and may explain the test’s high negative predictive value of 90%. Saccade testing can be repeated several times, and it is crucial not to browbeat the patient. In this context, it is also imperative that the patient have at least 2 s to rest on one target before refixating the other target. This allows the patient with a non-organic visual-field loss to localise the target in the supposedly blind retinal eccentricity.

Especially when used with a masked third observer, the test may be a tool in fast and objective decision-making when a non-organic aetiology of visual-field restriction is suspected.

Our test, however, also has several limitations and shortcomings. First, the test cannot be used in patients unable to hold fixation of the presented stimulus due to extremely decreased bilateral central visual acuity or nystagmus. In our study, all the patients were able to fixate and had sufficient visual acuity in at least one eye to detect the stimulus. Second, differentiating between absolute concentric non-organic visual-field loss and concentric visual-field loss with relative scotomas may present a problem. This has not been tested in this study. Patients with a relative concentric scotoma may be able to perform normal saccades towards the presented stimulus and thus may be mistaken as malingerers. In addition, the fact that the principal examiner was aware of the final diagnosis could have affected the course of the examination in unforeseen ways.

Although this method has a very high specifity in the diagnosis of non-organic visual-field defects, it should only be used as an extension of the complete neurological and neuro-ophthalmological examination to rule out an underlying organic component. Regarding the sensitivity of 87% of this test, the possibility that some patients with non-organic visual-field defects might be able to feign an erratic saccade pattern has to be kept in mind. Often referral to an ophthalmological clinic for specific testing depending on the nature of the visual-field loss and complaints of the patient might be necessary to confirm the diagnosis.

REFERENCES

Footnotes

  • Funding The study was partly funded by the OPOS Foundation, St Gallen.

  • Competing interests None.

  • Provenance and Peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.

  • Ethics approval Ethics approval was provided by Ethikkommission Kantonsspital St Gallen.